- From: Katie Haritos-Shea <ryladog@gmail.com>
- Date: Sat, 21 Sep 2019 12:55:47 +0900
- To: Andrew Somers <me@andysomers.com>
- Cc: Wayne Dick <wayneedick@gmail.com>, Jim Allan <jimallan@tsbvi.edu>, public-low-vision-a11y-tf <public-low-vision-a11y-tf@w3.org>
- Message-ID: <CAEy-OxHM8TgSupH87jsWEXtEPWcfaEAY=WjL1=nXJsFcs+=OpQ@mail.gmail.com>
Please be open-minded and polite in comments and responses. On Sat, Sep 21, 2019, 12:40 PM Andrew Somers <me@andysomers.com> wrote: > Hi Wayne. What are you doing? > > *No, my information is not out of date.* > > Wayne, you're referencing things out of context when you don't even know > what the context is. It is beyond infuriating, and your assertions are > meritless. > > The information in my post that you are attacking is based on *current* > international standards, and the most recent research. > > *But here's the really funny thing* (at least I'm trying to laugh as > opposed to being angry) the paper that you're claiming is most recent: *"Does > Print Size Matter” **Legge, Bigelow *is from 2011 and it is a *REVIEW > paper of the last 40 years of research* and not new experimental > research! So please don't try to make some spurious claim that it's somehow > superseding previous research, when in fact it is a *review* of previous > research. You're an academic, you should know better. > > But here’s the most hilarious part: I read and cite Legge et.al. > frequently,* INCLUDING this exact paper.* I read it over three months > ago. In fact It's one of the papers that will end up in the bibliography. > And it’s the paper that led to the work we are doing in terms of aligning > font metrics based on x-heights among other things. > > I actually read this paper, and I actually understand it. However I’m not > sure you do as you made several meritless statements deriding the very > rough draft of an outline that I had posted (and which I posted for an > unrelated issue). Your statements indicate a misunderstanding of the > underlying material. While of course your comments were infuriating to say > the least, I am trying to take a proactive approach to explain the body of > knowledge to you and hopefully correct misunderstandings for you and anyone > else reading. If you realize > > > *I’m Not Researching Printed Books.* > And printed material is not really what I am talking about. That posted > outline is unrelated to any of your comments. Nevertheless, as I stated in > the post,* it is a very ROUGH draft and it is NOT a published report.* I > posted it in response to a user that was making a lot of unfounded > assertions. My intent was to (hopefully) give him a little more > information as to what issues are at stake as that user was clearly > misunderstanding a lot of things and stirring things up as opposed to being > constructive. > > And importantly, I was never referring to* "16 inches to the stimulus"*. > That is more generally about reading printed words on paper, such as in a > book. The research I am doing involves emissive displays (computer monitor) > — and 16 inches is not necessarily the appropriate distance for displays, > but still that has nothing to do with what I was discussing in that post. > > Again, you are way off base and out of context. At that instant I was > discussing *comparative acuity levels*. That would be the meaning of the > line were I say: > >> >> - *Classification of Acuity can be divided into three broad groups:* >> >> Notice I did not say critical print size, though we do have original > research on that pending. > > *Wayne Said: *Here is the math. The normal acuity limit at 16 in. is > around 4.5pt with really clean eye chart font. Double that an you get 9pt, > newspaper size. 2.5 of that is is 11-12pt rounding. Now if you have 20/40 > vision your critical print size at 16 in. is 9pt. Double is 18pt and 2.5 > times is 22pt. at 16in. Of course 20/160 requires 8 times that. That is > hard to do even digitally, but 4 times at 1/2 distance is effectively 800%. > > > ??? Did you read this paragraph before sending? Didn't you say you were a > mathematician? For a mathematician this paragraph is very unclear. > Regardless not only do this and your other paragraphs have nothing to do > with my post, it demonstrates a misunderstanding of several key concepts > regarding research on reading speed which I hope to clarify below. BTW > where did you get the numbers you're quoting? Because those are not from > the cited Legge paper, nor are they in my post, *and in any event they > are not correct* so I am assuming they are misreferenced. > > > *LACK OF UNDERSTANDING* > I suspect you don’t understand when acuity level is being referenced vs > critical print size, because you are apparently confusing the two. You are > also misunderstanding the nature of research stimulus sizes vs practical > glyph sizes and font metrics. At any rate your math is using wrong values > and wrong assumptions. > > For the record, I was not using 16” as the stimulus distance in anything I > mentioned in my post. That’s something I guess you saw in the Legge paper. > Further the cited acuity figures you indicate are incorrect, as are the > assumptions regarding increasing the stimulus size, as are your > misconceptions of critical print size. > > But to help us get on the same page, let’s start with stimulus distance. > That paper was focused on reading, and mostly reviewed studies of reading > printed materials (though some were indeed monitor based). 16” is a > reasonable standard distance for a book or magazine, but not for a desktop > monitor (22” to 40”), and not for a small mobile device (often as close as > 4” to 5” to as far as 24”). 28” is a more useful monitor distance, and a > more commonly used length. Since Acuity is based on visual angle, that > means the physical font sizes will be different for the longer or shorter > distance, etc. > > FWIW The measurement that's important in terms of research is visual > angle. The visual angles that we are working with are essentially the same > in the cited Legge paper and other research. But visual angle is what > defines the CSS reference pixel: > > From W3C: > > The *CSS **reference pixel* is the visual angle of one pixel on a device > with a pixel density of 96dpi and a distance from the reader of an arm’s > length. For a nominal arm’s length of 28 inches, the visual angle is > therefore about 0.0213 degrees. For reading at arm’s length, 1px thus > corresponds to about 0.26 mm (1/96 inch). > > > > > *PLAIN LANGUAGE* > The outline I posted is up a work in progress for organizing some plain > language ideas for functional needs, again, very rough. An idea behind > plain language is to use only terms that are common knowledge. Visual angle > is not plain language, but “20/20” in the US is fairly well understood as > “normal” vision, as are the common font metrics like size. So the font > sizes I mentioned are what people commonly know: the full point size, in *CSS > it is called font-size* *(in the old days of lead type, called the point > body, also referred to as the type size, see left of below diagram).* > > > > *RESEARCH IS DIFFERENT* > “CSS font-size” is not what is used in reading speed research. The problem > with using “font-size” for research as a way to measure fonts is that it is > the entire font height from the top of the capitals to the bottom of a > lower case descender, plus whatever built-in leading there may be. For > vision research, we need a much more consistent and accurate measure, so we > use a specific glyph, and one of the most common, and the one used in the > cited Legge paper is called* x-height*. *(See the right of the diagram) * > > x-height is the height from the baseline to the top of the lower case x. > x-height was mentioned prominently in that paper because nearly all the > font measurements for nearly all the referenced experiments indicated size > by *x-height* and not font-size. For Times, a print font used throughout > much research including the cited paper, the x-height is 0.45 of the total > font-size. The fonts more commonly used on a computers display such as > Verdana, tend to have an x-height of about 0.55 of the total font-size. > > Using Times as an example, this means that if the lower case x-height is > 4.5 points, then the actual font-size is 10 points. > > > *MATH IS FUN* > > *5 arc-minutes is the letter-acuity level for 20/20 — that is the > specified definition.* > > What this means in a practical sense is the entire height of the letter > *E* subtends 5 arc minutes of visual angle. > > When we are researching reading speed, we usually are using lowercase > letters, then we use the lowercase *x-height* set to subtend five arc > minutes of visual angle for the 20/20 letter acuity level as the baseline > reference. > > There are 60 arc-minutes in one degree, thus 5 arc-minutes equals 0.08333° > *MATH: *5/60 = 0.08333 > > At 16 inches, visual angle 0.08333° equals 0.02327” as the x-height. > *MATH* *simplified method, usable for small angles:* (16 * 0.08333) / > (180 / pi) > > 0.02327” equals 1.68 points or 2.23 px for the 20/20 acuity limit x-height > at 16” viewing distance. > *MATH*: inches*72 = points *or* inches*96 = px > > Times x-height 1.68 pt equals 3.7 point font size (20/20 acuity limit) > Times x-height 2.23 px equals 5.0 px font-size (20/20 acuity limit) > *MATH: *x-height / 0.45 > > *ACUITY TO FONT SIZE CHART (BETA — 16” distance shown)* > > > The "PT" section relates to font size by point, the "PX" section relates > to font size by CSS pixels. > > The column under "PT" or"PX" is the *x-height*, which is at the acuity > level (five arc min or 20/20). > > > > *UNDERSTANDING STANDARDS* > > As a point of reference, the common standards for display minimum text > size for readability in ANSI, FAA, and others relates to *22 arc minutes* > of visual angle. > > As established, the acuity limit is five arc-minutes. > > Using our example of Times, a font size of 22 arc minutes would have an > x-height of 10 arc minutes. > > I don't think you have to be a mathematician to see that 10 arc minutes > happens to be twice five arc minutes. > > Twice. Well let's see what did that paper say a critical print size was? > Oh yeah, two times or more. > > > *ALREADY BUIL**T IN* > > You see Wayne, the paper you cited is not defining anything new. > Classical design concepts have essentially used critical print size going > back hundreds of years. And it's existed in the standards for a very long > time. You seem to think that everything in the past has been based on > minimum acuity size, but nothing could be farther from the truth. > > The font standards for displays, and for reading material, etc. are not > based on minimum acuity–they're based on readability. In other words the > essence of critical print size is already built in as a factor of good > design. > > *And to dispel a final misunderstanding* > In the chart above, you’ll notice that some numbers of font size are red — > this is because you can't just keep increasing the font size to increase > reading speed. At a point, text gets too large and reading speed once again > will start to decrease. > > This goes back to what I've been saying for the last several months about > how human perception needs fit within the range. And it also points to > problems with severe impairments, in that while they may need to text to be > made very large so they can discern it, the fact that it is made very large > in itself interferes with their ability to read it quickly. This also > points to problems where zooming in text to help one person actually can > cause problems for someone with a different impairment. > > > *WAYNE: *The sizes suggested in WHO report will definitely result in size > induced slow reading and a serious error rate. > > > What WHO report? I’m not using it. The only reference in my post were the > previous WHO *categories*, and I'm using the older categories because > that's where it uses the term* "low vision"*. The new categories for the > same 20/70 - 20/200 level uses the term* "moderate"* I don't know about > you but I think low vision works better as a more descriptive term for the > general public. The term ”moderate” makes it seem less important, or less > impairing. > > The original work we are doing right now in terms of critical reading > speed related testing is ongoing, so I will not comment further on CFS. And > in the post did I discuss “critical print size” anywhere? No I did not, so > I really don’t know why you decided to rip into me about critical print > size, which you have just demonstrated you misunderstand. I did discuss > critical *CONTRAST* which is different. > > > *COMMUNICATION* > > In the future Wayne, when you want to rip some of my work apart > (especially unfinished rough drafts), you might consider asking me first so > that we make sure that we're on the same page, or at least so you > understand the context, particularly in connection with what is clearly > stated as a very rough draft. > > Whenever you have asked me questions in the past, I have given you a > lengthy and in-depth response. When ever you asked what was going on a I've > tried to read you in and keep you abreast of the status when I was able. > > So why is it that you seem to be interested in interfering, blocking, and > disparaging my efforts and research? Because that has been your tone & > attitude toward me since your very first post to me in April. Please stop. > > > > Regards, > > Andy > > > > > > > On Sep 19, 2019, at 3:14 PM, Wayne Dick <wayneedick@gmail.com> wrote: > > Hi Jim, > > This information is out of date. The report may have been published > recently, but the background research is old. > > The critical print size is about 2 times to 2 and 1/2 times the acuity > limit. [legge, Bigelow, "Does Print Size Matter"]. This is true for normal > and low vision. This means that 20/50 requires about 250% enlargement. 2/60 > requires 300% etc. for critical print size. If the person has central > retina damage the magnification factor is greater. > > Here is the math. The normal acuity limit at 16 in. is around 4.5pt with > really clean eye chart font. Double that an you get 9pt, newspaper size. > 2.5 of that is is 11-12pt rounding. Now if you have 20/40 vision your > critical print size at 16 in. is 9pt. Double is 18pt and 2.5 times is > 22pt. at 16in. Of course 20/160 requires 8 times that. That is hard to do > even digitally, but 4 times at 1/2 distance is effectively 800%. > > The critical print size is the minimum size a person can read at optimum > speed and error rate. The sizes suggested in WHO report will definitely > result in size induced slow reading and a serious error rate. These > numbers look like the kind of pre-digital data that was used for 1.4.4. We > need to stop that old data. When print was on paper, 200% was all anyone > could achieve. A pamphlet enlarged to 400% would become a small > encyclopedia. We can do lots better today with digital data in markup > language. > > Best, Wayne > > On Thu, Sep 19, 2019 at 12:51 PM Jim Allan <jimallan@tsbvi.edu> wrote: > >> This comes from the AG (WCAG) list. Passing along for consideration. >> >> ---------- Forwarded message --------- >> From: Andrew Somers <notifications@github.com> >> Date: Tue, Sep 17, 2019 at 9:08 PM >> Subject: Re: [w3c/wcag] Proposal for color and contrast (1.3.1, 1.4.1, >> 1.4.3., 1.4.6, 1.4.11) (WCAG 3.0) (#901) >> To: w3c/wcag <wcag@noreply.github.com> >> Cc: Subscribed <subscribed@noreply.github.com> >> >> <snip> >> A BRIEF ENCAPSULATION OF USER VISUAL NEEDS: *Visual Acuity deficits:* >> >> Acuity is essentially the ability to resolve a stimuli in the eye and >> perceive it in focus. “Blurryness” is the plain language way to describe >> poor acuity. >> >> - A _primary_way to assist visual acuity is corrective refraction >> (glasses/contacts) which is outside scope. In terms of display or design, >> and for all other things being equal, acuity is assisted by the >> appropriate _SIZE_which needs to be within a range (not too small but also >> not too big) for best perception. >> - Some causes of acuity loss, such as cataracts, require surgery to >> correct. >> - Classification of Acuity can be divided into three broad groups: >> - *20/10 thru 20/63:* normal through near-normal. Existing >> standards tend to be built around this range, which relates to a font size >> of 12pt on the printed page. This serves as a “baseline” or foundation from >> which stronger accessibility needs can be defined. 20/30 is the lowest >> acuity for a private pilot, and 20/40 is the lowest for non-commercial >> drivers in most states. >> - *20/70 thru 20/200:* Low Vision, per the WHO definition. If a >> font at 100% size is good for 20/63, then if you double the size to 200% >> (24pt), you accomodate 20/150. To accomodate 20/200, then increase size to >> 275% (33pt). >> - *Above 20/200:* Legally blind. 20/400 needs 550% larger size >> (66pt). >> - *Discuss* size adjust (user) and design minimums. And accommodating >> user changes without breaking content, etc. (methods). >> >> *Contrast Sensitivity deficits:* >> >> Contrast Sensitivity Function (CSF) can be impacted by poor acuity, by >> retinal disease such as AMD, retinal migrains, by degraded ocular media >> (cataract, etc), and by neurological problems (MS, neuropathy). *VERY >> ROUGH (to be written):* >> >> - CSF deficits caused due to poor acuity (blurry vision) is typically >> helped best by addressing the acuity issues when possible. >> - CSF is directly linked to spatial frequency (i.e. size), especially >> closer to threshold. >> - Increasing stimulus size will increase perceived contrast (within a >> range). >> - A key aspect of stimulus size is the stroke width of a font (i.e. >> font “weight”) — Increasing a font’s size increases perceived contrast, but >> largely due to the increase of stroke width as rendered to the screen. >> Stroke width is the aspect of a font that most closely follows Michelson >> Contrast (gratings). >> - Aging ocular media (lens, cornea, vitreous) can affect contrast, >> but moreover these can cause problems with glare which reduces perceived >> contrast, while simultaneously being made worse as stimulus contrast >> increases. >> - Intraocular glare reduces or obscures perceived contrast, but >> contrast perception is improved by _reducing the contrast _of what is being >> viewed. >> - Put another way, higher contrast objects cause more glare which >> reduces the “contrast legibility” versus lower contrast objects that cause >> less glare. The extreme example is headlights from an oncoming car at night. >> - *TBD Discuss: luminance contrast, threshold vs supra and critical >> contrast levels. Discuss design contrast. Discuss display luminance adjust >> (user). Discuss polarity.* >> - Contrast Sensitivity Function is typically measured with a >> Pelli-Robson style of chart, which measures the “just noticeable >> difference” or threshold of visibility. >> - A Pelli-Robson score of 2 indicates “perfect normal vision >> contrast” which equates to a contrast of 1% (i.e. 1.01 to 1 ) >> - A score of 1.5 is a noticeable degrading of CSF, and equates to >> a contrast of 3% (i.e. 1.03 to 1) >> - A score of 1 is a serious contrast impairment, and equates to a >> threshold contrast of 10% (1.1 to 1) >> - These are a measure of the point where a stimuli becomes >> visible, which is useful in a clinical setting for detecting disease, but >> do not indicate the level of “critical contrast” where an item is “most >> readable.” >> >> *Visual Field deficits:* >> >> Closely related/essentially part of contrast sensitivity impairments are >> those relating to visual field. >> >> - Central vision loss is a loss of vision in the fovea (central >> vision) forcing these users to learn to read using their peripheral vision. >> - Peripheral blindness, or narrowing of the visual field (aka tunnel >> vision), >> - Makes it harder to notice changes in content (i.e. a warning >> message) outside of the area the user is looking directly at. >> >> *Color Vision deficits:* >> >> Color Vision Deficiency (protan, deutan, tritan CVD types) is primarily >> helped by ensuring there is enough luminance contrast between items (i.e. >> between text and a background, or between roadways on a map and geographic >> features on the map). >> >> - Also, ensure that color is not used as the sole means of providing >> information (that is, don’t rely on “red” as a color that means “stop” — >> descriptive text of symbols are also needed to communicate meaning.) >> - Protanopia (red deficient) may have problems with some monitor >> types (such as UHD/Rec2020) the red primary is close to the cut off for the >> green cone and is perceived much darker.. *(need plain language for >> this)* >> - sRGB monitors are recommended for Protanopia as the red primary is >> within the green cone sensitivity. The protan will see this red a little >> darker, which should be considered in calculating contrast. >> - The rare monochromats are also aided by luminance contrast, though >> may need to set the display to a monochrome mode, and have control over >> luminance and ambient illumination (such as for rod monochromacy). >> >> *Cognitive/Neurological related Visual Deficits:* >> >> 62% of the brain is involved in visual processing. Over 20% of the brain >> is dedicated to visual processing, and of 42% processes visual in >> conjunction with other senses such as auditory and tactile. >> >> - The other impairment types above are mostly associated with the eye >> itself, these are associated with processing the signals from the eye. >> - Someone who had a stroke, and the stroke damaged some part of >> vision processing may have a problem with only that aspect of vision. For >> instance, if the motion detection part of the brain is damaged, they may >> see a car that is parked, but when the car moves it “disappears” in that >> the brain ignores it/it is nor “perceived” (“ *visual neglect* ”) >> - With *agnosia,* the visual pathways and brain are capable >> of _seeing_objects or people, but cannot _recognize_them. >> - Cognitive impairments, brain damage (from stroke or other incident) >> can also cause some of the functional problems normally associated with >> ocular impairments, such as blurred vision, field loss, light sensitivity, >> hallucinations, etc. >> - Ocular migraines can directly interfere with vision by introducing >> “blockage” to vision, such as with ocular migraines auras, which can appear >> as zig zags in the vision, “seeing stars”, etc. >> >> In Closing >> >> Some of things you mention in your initial issue are fairly well >> understood, and in fact making their way into either SC extensions for 2.2, >> or new standards and guidelines for Silver. (One example is font weight, as >> those proposed SCs are already being created). >> >> However, you make statements that are not supported by research, your >> codepen notwithstanding. Visual perception is not binary logic, so >> "absolute" statements don't really fly in a field where there *are no >> absolutes.* Human perception is far more complex than can be determined >> by some examples at maximum contrast. >> >> I have listed references and footnotes to authoritative research >> supporting most of my posts in #695 >> <https://github.com/w3c/wcag/issues/695> and elsewhere, and I do suggest >> reading through those references to gain a better understanding of the >> underlying concepts. In particular you might want to read Legge's book >> Psychophysics of Vision. >> >> And please keep in mind these standards are ultimately about the >> functional needs of a very wide swath of users & impairments. It is needs >> that should be considered, not so much the abstraction layer methods. >> >> Regards, >> >> Andy >> >> Andrew Somers >> WAI Invited Expert >> >> >> *Color Science Research Silver Task Force Low Vision Task Force* >> >> — >> You are receiving this because you are subscribed to this thread. >> Reply to this email directly, view it on GitHub >> <https://github.com/w3c/wcag/issues/901?email_source=notifications&email_token=ABX5MLYG5JNF6RKSCJG4XVLQKGEQNA5CNFSM4IW5LFSKYY3PNVWWK3TUL52HS4DFVREXG43VMVBW63LNMVXHJKTDN5WW2ZLOORPWSZGOD66RWRA#issuecomment-532486980>, >> or mute the thread >> <https://github.com/notifications/unsubscribe-auth/ABX5ML4PMVIMF6MDCMNIXULQKGEQNANCNFSM4IW5LFSA> >> . >> >> >> -- >> Jim Allan, Accessibility Coordinator >> Texas School for the Blind and Visually Impaired >> 1100 W. 45th St., Austin, Texas 78756 >> voice 512.206.9315 fax: 512.206.9452 http://www.tsbvi.edu/ >> "We shape our tools and thereafter our tools shape us." McLuhan, 1964 >> > >
Attachments
- image/png attachment: Anatomy-of-Typography.png
- image/png attachment: Screen_Shot_2019-09-20_at_6.33.31_PM.png
Received on Saturday, 21 September 2019 03:56:27 UTC